PSY 368 Human Memory Processing Announcements Craik and

  • Slides: 34
Download presentation
PSY 368 Human Memory Processing

PSY 368 Human Memory Processing

Announcements • Craik and Lockhart (1972) download, read, and answer focus questions, due Monday

Announcements • Craik and Lockhart (1972) download, read, and answer focus questions, due Monday Feb 27 • Processing views homework due Web Feb 29

Structural Model • Memory is also made up of Processes • Memory composed of

Structural Model • Memory is also made up of Processes • Memory composed of storage structures that hold memories for a period of time • Sensory memory • Short-term memory (STM) • Long-term memory (LTM)

4 Structure Processes • Structural (Modal) views • Properties of memorized information (e. g.

4 Structure Processes • Structural (Modal) views • Properties of memorized information (e. g. , how long it lasts) come from where it is stored (Sensor, STM, or LTM) • E. g. , the longer an item is held in STM, the more likely it would get into LTM (and be stored more ‘durably’) • We’ve talked about things like rehearsal and elaboration as being important for moving information between the different memory systems • Processing views • Focus on what gets done to the information • e. g. , rote rehearsal versus elaborative rehearsal • Need to look at both learning (encoding) and at test (retrieval)

Structure Processes • Processing views • Encoding (focus on ‘getting things into memory’) •

Structure Processes • Processing views • Encoding (focus on ‘getting things into memory’) • Level of processing • Interaction of Encoding & Retrieval (focus on ‘getting things in and out of memory’) • Transfer-appropriate processing • Importance of context • Encoding specificity principle

Demo For each item below with a ! next to it, indicate if it

Demo For each item below with a ! next to it, indicate if it is alive. For each item below with a * next to it, indicate if it has an “e. ” ! flag ! kite * house * jail ! spider * snake * picture ! duck * flame ! bottle ! tree * happy * storm ! ball ! class * horse ! zebra * test ! window * hair * dream ! rock ! table * fountain

7 Rehearsal Mechanic (1964) • Task: • Participants had to repeat nonsense syllables either

7 Rehearsal Mechanic (1964) • Task: • Participants had to repeat nonsense syllables either once or many times. • Only half of the participants were warned of an upcoming recall test. • Conclusion: • Rehearsal led to better recall • Knowing that there is a test coming prompted additional processing in the intentional learning group. • Results:

8 Rehearsal Glenberg, Smith, and Green (1977) • Task: • Participants were asked to

8 Rehearsal Glenberg, Smith, and Green (1977) • Task: • Participants were asked to remember numbers over a delay. • During the delay, they had to read out words (in time with tones every 2 sec. ) • Some words were repeated only once during the delay; others were repeated many times. • Participants then recalled the numbers followed by a surprise recall (or recognition) test for the words. • Results: • Having nine times as many repetitions only increased recall by 1. 5% (9% for recognition), suggesting that simple maintenance rehearsal doesn’t help long-term recall much.

9 Rehearsal Mechanic (1964) • Finding: • Maintenance did help in one condition •

9 Rehearsal Mechanic (1964) • Finding: • Maintenance did help in one condition • Stimuli: • Unfamiliar stimuli that need to be learned from scratch • Rationale: • Repeating unfamiliar stimuli with no natural links between them boosts their representation in phonological LTM Glenberg, et al. (1977) • Finding: • Maintenance didn’t help • Stimuli: • Already known words • Rationale: • The recall test relied on meaningful links between the known words (already in LTM), which depend on deeper, semantic features

Demo For each item below with a ! next to it, indicate if it

Demo For each item below with a ! next to it, indicate if it is alive. For each item below with a * next to it, indicate if it has an “e. ” ! flag ! kite * house * jail ! spider * snake * picture ! duck * flame ! bottle ! tree * happy * storm ! ball ! class * horse ! zebra * test ! window * hair * dream ! rock ! table * fountain Write down as many of the words as you can remember. • Count up how many were from the “with an e” and how many from “is it alive? ”

Levels of Processing • Emphasizes memory processes rather than structures • Based on the

Levels of Processing • Emphasizes memory processes rather than structures • Based on the idea that the strength of a memory trace is determined by how the original info was processed • Incoming stimuli pass through a series of analyzing mechanisms • Memory traces are a product of how stimuli are analyzed • Strength of trace depends on: • Attention paid to stimulus • Depth of processing carried out • Connections with existing knowledge

Level of Processing Craik & Lockhart (1972) • Argue against structural/modal models of memory

Level of Processing Craik & Lockhart (1972) • Argue against structural/modal models of memory • Features that distinguish stores are not clear • Distinguished elaborative and maintenance rehearsal • Elaborative - rehearse item using its meaning (better memory) • Maintenance - rehearse items by repeating it over and over • Keep in STM/WM, but doesn’t get it into LTM • Importance of Incidental learning • If participants know that they’ll be tested for memory they may engage in different techniques/strategies

Level of Processing Craik & Lockhart (1972) • Considered level of processing at study

Level of Processing Craik & Lockhart (1972) • Considered level of processing at study to be more important for memory than intent to learn • Levels of processing = how “deeply” the item is processed • The depth of processing helps determine the durability in LTM. Level of Processing Example SHALLOW DEEP 1) Visual Form “DOG” includes the letters D, O, and G 2) Phonology Rhymes with FOG 3) Semantics (Meaning) A four-legged pet that often chases cats and chews on bones

Levels of Processing Craik and Tulving (1975) Task: • Participants viewed words and were

Levels of Processing Craik and Tulving (1975) Task: • Participants viewed words and were asked to make three different types of judgments: • Visual processing (e. g. “Is LOG in upper case? ” Y/N) • Phonological (e. g. “Does DOG rhyme with LOG? ” Y/N) • Semantic (e. g. “Does DOG fit in the sentence: ‘The ___ chased the cat’? ” Y/N) • Finally, participants were asked to recognize the words they had seen before in a surprise test including both old and new words.

Levels of Processing Craik and Tulving (1975) Results: • Words that were more deeply

Levels of Processing Craik and Tulving (1975) Results: • Words that were more deeply processed were more easily recognized -- particularly for questions with a “YES” response. Conclusions: • Deeper processed items had stronger memory traces than shallow processed items. • “Yes” responses were better recalled because these items are better integrated with the encoding question.

Levels of Processing Hyde and Jenkins (1973) • Auditory study of words with incidental

Levels of Processing Hyde and Jenkins (1973) • Auditory study of words with incidental and intentional tasks • Half knew they be tested later • Half didn’t know about a later test • Control group: no instruction, but told about later memory test • Task: studied 24 common words, auditory presentation, free recall task SHALLOW (1) Look for “e” and “g” in word DEEP (2) Identify part of speech (3) Rate how common the word is (4) Rate pleasantness of word

Levels of Processing Hyde and Jenkins (1973) Results • Depth of processing effect •

Levels of Processing Hyde and Jenkins (1973) Results • Depth of processing effect • Incidental: the deeper the level of processing, the more words recalled. • Incidental: recall on the pleasantness task was as good as control group recall.

Levels of Processing Craik and Watkins (1973) • Tested the idea that rehearsal doesn't

Levels of Processing Craik and Watkins (1973) • Tested the idea that rehearsal doesn't really help memory. • Had people rehearse a word until another word that started with the same letter occurred in the list. • Task: report the last word that began with that letter. • Rehearse words that start with "g” daughter, oil, rifle, garden, grain, table, anchor, football, giraffe Operational definition of rehearsal time: the number of intervening words garden = 0 grain = 3 Rehearsal time ranged from 0 to 12.

Levels of Processing Craik and Watkins (1973) • Tested the idea that rehearsal doesn't

Levels of Processing Craik and Watkins (1973) • Tested the idea that rehearsal doesn't really help memory. • Had people rehearse a word until another word that started with the same letter occurred in the list. • Task: report the last word that began with that letter. • 10 minutes after the final trial, subjects got a surprise recall test: report as many of the words that began with "g”. daughter, oil, rifle, garden, grain, table, anchor, football, giraffe Results: • Recall of an item was not correlated with how long it had been rehearsed. • garden remembered just as well as grain

Levels of Processing Criticisms: • It is difficult to operationally define depth of processing.

Levels of Processing Criticisms: • It is difficult to operationally define depth of processing. • Can’t use processing speed to define it. • Different levels of processing can occur simultaneously, rather than in series, making them hard to separate in a task. • Deeper processing does not always lead to better performance. • Ignores effects of retrieval task

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) • Focus on processing at study (encoding)

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) • Focus on processing at study (encoding) AND test (retrieval) • If processing at study and test match, better memory • Levels of Processing effect for tasks that use a lot of semantic processing (like free recall)

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) • Task: • Participants made either a

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) • Task: • Participants made either a phonological or semantic judgment about each item on a word list. • Study: eagle (yes/no fits clue) • Deep - The ____ is the US national bird. • Shallow - rhymes with legal • The learning was incidental: participants were not told that they would have to later recall the words. • This constrains (limits) the learning strategies used.

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) • Task: • The final test was

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) • Task: • The final test was either: • A standard recognition test for the learned words. • A rhyming recognition test for learned words • e. g. , Was a word presented that rhymed with “regal”?

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) Encoding: Recognition Rhyming test: Does ____ rhyme

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) Encoding: Recognition Rhyming test: Does ____ rhyme 63% with legal? (eagle) 49% Does ____ have 84% feathers? (eagle) 33% • Results: • Standard recognition test: Deeper processing led to better performance. • Rhyming recognition test: The shallower rhyme-based encoding task led to better performance because it matched the demands of the testing situation.

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) Encoding: Recognition Rhyming test: Does ____ rhyme

Transfer-appropriate processing Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) Encoding: Recognition Rhyming test: Does ____ rhyme 63% with legal? (eagle) 49% Does ____ have 84% feathers? (eagle) 33% • Conclusion: • The take-home message is that when the processing at encoding matches the processing at retrieval, performance will be better. • It only makes sense to talk about a learning method’s efficiency in the context of the type of final test.

Context Effects Abernathy (1940) • Found that students tested in the same classroom in

Context Effects Abernathy (1940) • Found that students tested in the same classroom in which they learned did better on tests than students tested in a different classroom. • Context matters • Interaction of encoding and retrieval • Context at study (e. g. , cues, environment, processing, etc. ) and context at retrieval • Better memory if the contexts match

Context Effects Godden and Baddeley (1975) • Used subjects from a SCUBA diving club

Context Effects Godden and Baddeley (1975) • Used subjects from a SCUBA diving club in gear Study Condition Test Condition On Land Underwater On Land Match Mismatch Match Underwater

Context Effects Godden and Baddeley (1975) • Used subjects from a SCUBA diving club

Context Effects Godden and Baddeley (1975) • Used subjects from a SCUBA diving club in gear Results: • Recall was better when study environment and test environment matched, regardless of whether it was on land or under water.

Context Effects • Also called state-dependent memory • Match between study and test state/context

Context Effects • Also called state-dependent memory • Match between study and test state/context predicts memory performance • Goodwin et al. (1969) alcohol study: • Some subjects received 10 oz of 80 -proof vodka at study, at test, or both (errors) Study Condition Results • For items learned during Test intoxication, there is a large Condition difference in performance at recall sober • For items learned during sober, the drunk difference is smaller sober drunk 1. 25 4. 58 2. 25 2. 50

Context Effects • For emotional state: mood-dependent memory • Bartlett & Santrock (1979) mood

Context Effects • For emotional state: mood-dependent memory • Bartlett & Santrock (1979) mood study: • happy or neutral mood at study • happy or neutral mood at test • Recall was better when moods matched • This effect is not always replicated -- one important factor is to produce a strong, sincere, mood.

Encoding Specificity Principle Tulving (1972, 1983) • The recollection of an event or a

Encoding Specificity Principle Tulving (1972, 1983) • The recollection of an event or a certain aspect of it depends on the interaction between the properties of the encoded event and the properties of the retrieval information. • What is stored determines what retrieval cues are effective in providing access to what is stored. • Match between cues at study and test results in better memory • The most effective retrieval cues will be the ones you used during the original learning.

Encoding Specificity Principle Thompson and Tulving (1970) • Examined effectiveness of cue • Had

Encoding Specificity Principle Thompson and Tulving (1970) • Examined effectiveness of cue • Had people learn lists of strong or weak associates. • Strong vs. weak cues (“flower”) • Strong: bloom • Weak: fruit • Study: no cue vs. weak cue • Test: no cue, weak cue, or strong cue

Encoding Specificity Principle Thompson and Tulving (1970) • The best retrieval cue for a

Encoding Specificity Principle Thompson and Tulving (1970) • The best retrieval cue for a word like “flower” would be a strong associate like “bloom. ” “fruit” is weakly associated to “flower, ” and would be unlikely to pull it out. • Thompson and Tulving showed that this can be reversed if you change the study context.

Brief Summary • Where something is stored isn’t everything. How you encode and retrieve

Brief Summary • Where something is stored isn’t everything. How you encode and retrieve the information is critical. • Encoding: • Levels of processing • Encoding + Retrieval • Context effects (environment, mood) • Transfer-appropriate processing • Encoding specificity principle (cues)