The CIE System Dr Huw Owens Dr Huw

  • Slides: 27
Download presentation
The CIE System Dr Huw Owens © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester

The CIE System Dr Huw Owens © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester :

Introduction • • • Lights, Surfaces and Observers Colour Matching Experiments The CIE System

Introduction • • • Lights, Surfaces and Observers Colour Matching Experiments The CIE System Tristimulus Values Chromaticity Diagrams Dominant Wavelength and Purity © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 2

 • • • Standing in line to See the show tonight And there's

• • • Standing in line to See the show tonight And there's a light of Heavy glow By the way I tried to say I'd be there waiting for Dani's the girl is Singing songs to me Beneath the marquee Overload © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 3

The CIE System • Specifying colour can be difficult. Even if a physical sample

The CIE System • Specifying colour can be difficult. Even if a physical sample is selected from a colour order system, its appearance will probably change with changes in illuminant and viewing conditions. • How can we simplify colour specification? Colour matching. • Colour matching first performed by Newton (1730). He found white light could be produced by mixing yellow and blue lights. • Colorimetry: A synthesis of two words, colour and metrein (Greek meaning to measure). It is the science of colour measurement. • Lovibond (1887) developed a device with which he could specify the colour of beer. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 4

Colour Perception • Requires three factors Observer Light Source Object © Dr Huw Owens

Colour Perception • Requires three factors Observer Light Source Object © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 5

Illuminants and Light Sources • A light source is a physical emitter of radiation

Illuminants and Light Sources • A light source is a physical emitter of radiation such as a candle or the sun. • An illuminant is a table of spectral power distributions; a blueprint for a possible light source. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 6

Standard Illuminants • In 1931 the CIE (Commision Internationale de l’Éclairage) defined 3 light

Standard Illuminants • In 1931 the CIE (Commision Internationale de l’Éclairage) defined 3 light sources by their SPD (relative power, 400 -700 nm). • A - a tungsten filament lamp • B - tungsten with yellow filter (sunlight) • C - tungsten with blue filter (daylight) B and C no longer used. • In 1964 the CIE introduced a further series of standards based on measurements of daylight and extended the spectral range. D 50, D 55, D 60, D 65 • D 65 used in textile industry as standard daylight No lamp available to emulate D 65 exactly. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 7

Illuminants © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 8

Illuminants © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 8

Surface Reflectance Spectra Wavelength (nm) %R 400 6. 1 420 6. 2 440 6.

Surface Reflectance Spectra Wavelength (nm) %R 400 6. 1 420 6. 2 440 6. 5 460 7. 1 480 7. 3 500 7. 2 520 7. 5 540 8. 1 560 8. 5 580 10. 2 600 22. 1 620 40. 1 640 58. 9 660 65. 5 680 70. 2 700 73. 4 © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 9

1924 CIE Standard Photopic Observer Brightness: Attribute of visual perception according to which an

1924 CIE Standard Photopic Observer Brightness: Attribute of visual perception according to which an area appears to emit, or reflect, more or less light (CIE 17. 4) © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 10

Grassmann’s Laws of Additive Colour Matching • Symmetry Law – If colour stimulus A

Grassmann’s Laws of Additive Colour Matching • Symmetry Law – If colour stimulus A matches colour stimulus B, the colour stimulus B matches colour stimulus A. • Transitivity Law – then A matches C. If A matches B and B matches C • Proportionality Law – if A matches B, then αA matches αB, where α is any positive factor by which the radiant power of the colour stimulus is increased or reduced, while its relative spectral distribution is kept the same. • Additivity Law – If A, B, C, D are any four colour stimuli, then if any two of the following three colour matches: • A matches B, C matches D, and • (A+C) matches (B+D), then • (A+D) matches (B+C) © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 11

Primaries for the 1931 Colour Matching Experiment © Dr Huw Owens - University of

Primaries for the 1931 Colour Matching Experiment © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 12

Colour-Matching Experiment © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 13

Colour-Matching Experiment © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 13

Test Field Admixture of three primaries © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester

Test Field Admixture of three primaries © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : Reference Field Admixture of three primaries

RGB colour-matching functions • CIE colour-matching experiment Wright’s observers’ results Blue Green © Dr

RGB colour-matching functions • CIE colour-matching experiment Wright’s observers’ results Blue Green © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 Red 15

The Standard Observer • The CIE defined the colour vision of the ‘average’ human

The Standard Observer • The CIE defined the colour vision of the ‘average’ human by asking a panel of observers to match the monochromatic spectral colours with red, green and blue lights, having first ‘balanced’ them to match a standard ‘white’ lamp. • In 1931 the CIE converted 2 sets of results, Wright’s (10 observers) and Guild’s (7 observers), so that the white and the 3 primaries were the same. They used the equal energy white (illuminant E) and 3 monochromatic primaries. However, this still meant that at any wavelength the amount of one of the primaries was negative. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 16

RGB to XYZ colour-matching functions • Judd suggested to the CIE that new ‘supersaturated’

RGB to XYZ colour-matching functions • Judd suggested to the CIE that new ‘supersaturated’ primaries be defined so as to avoid negative values. These were called X (red), Y (green) and Z (blue). They were defined by drawing a triangle which was outside the spectrum locus and the RGB triangle. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 17

The Standard Observer • The standard observer is a set of colour-matching functions. These

The Standard Observer • The standard observer is a set of colour-matching functions. These were obtained in 1931 for a 2 degree field of view. • In 1964 the CIE introduced a new set of colourmatching functions and these were obtained using a larger (10 degree) field of view. • CIE 1931 2 degree CIE 1964 10 degree • In 1964 the CIE also supplemented the original set of illuminants (A, B and C) with a new set of illuminants based upon the spectral power distributions (SPDs) of a blackbody radiator – these are the D illuminants. D 65 is the spectral power distribution of a blackbody radiator at 6500 K and is used to simulate daylight. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 18

XYZ colour-matching functions © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 19

XYZ colour-matching functions © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 19

Computing Tristimulus Values λ E(λ) P(λ) Y(λ) E(λ)P(λ)Y(λ) 400 90 0. 8 0. 01

Computing Tristimulus Values λ E(λ) P(λ) Y(λ) E(λ)P(λ)Y(λ) 400 90 0. 8 0. 01 0. 72 420 85 0. 83 0. 02 1. 41 440 82 0. 84 0. 04 2. 76 99 0. 12 0. 01 0. 12 … 700 Tristimulus Value Illuminant Reflectance © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 Observer (colourmatching function) 20

Computing Tristimulus Values • k is a normalising factor, k = • This ensures

Computing Tristimulus Values • k is a normalising factor, k = • This ensures that Y is always 100 for a perfect reflecting diffuser for any illuminant. Note that X and Z do not necessarily sum to 100. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 21

Table of Weights Tables of weights are reproduced in many text books that pre-multiply

Table of Weights Tables of weights are reproduced in many text books that pre-multiply K, E, and X, Y, Z. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 22

Illuminant White Points Illuminant Observer X Y Z Equal Energy 1931 100. 0 Equal

Illuminant White Points Illuminant Observer X Y Z Equal Energy 1931 100. 0 Equal Enegy 1964 100. 0 A (1931) 109. 9 100. 0 35. 6 A (1964) 111. 1 100. 0 35. 2 D 65 (1931) 95. 0 100. 0 108. 9 D 65 (1964) 94. 8 100. 0 107. 3 TL 84 (F 11) 1931 101. 0 100. 0 64. 4 TL 84 (F 11) 1964 103. 8 100. 0 65. 6 © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 23

The CIE Chromaticity Diagram • This is a 2 -dimensional x, y chart which

The CIE Chromaticity Diagram • This is a 2 -dimensional x, y chart which gives an indication of colour, but removes lightness information. • This can be rectified by quoting the Y tristimulus value as well as the x, y coordinates. © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 24

Tristimulus Values • The XYZ values can easily be computed. They quantify the amounts

Tristimulus Values • The XYZ values can easily be computed. They quantify the amounts of the three imaginary primaries that an observer would use to match the stimulus if used in an additive mixture. They form a specification of the stimulus that takes into account the illumination, the surface and the observer. • However, they cannot be used to predict appearance! © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 25

The CIE Chromaticity Diagram © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021

The CIE Chromaticity Diagram © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 26

Dominant Wavelength and Purity © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021

Dominant Wavelength and Purity © Dr Huw Owens - University of Manchester : 2/28/2021 27