Why prioritise marked consonants Some research suggests we

  • Slides: 15
Download presentation
Why prioritise marked consonants? Some research suggests we should target MARKED properties in order

Why prioritise marked consonants? Some research suggests we should target MARKED properties in order to facilitate acquisition of unmarked aspects of the system. Markedness is a concept from the study of the sound systems of all natural languages. A marked feature in a language implies the necessary presence of another feature hence “implicational relationship”. Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

§ In markedness theory, in English, fricatives, the voiceless stops that occur in /s/

§ In markedness theory, in English, fricatives, the voiceless stops that occur in /s/ clusters (the adjuncts), affricates and clusters are ‘marked’. Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

1. 2. 3. 4. FRICATIVES are marked, implying stops. VOICED STOPS are marked, implying

1. 2. 3. 4. FRICATIVES are marked, implying stops. VOICED STOPS are marked, implying voiceless stops. AFFRICATES are marked, implying fricatives. CLUSTERS are marked, implying singletons. In intervention 1. Target fricatives to ‘get’ fricatives and stops. 2. Target the voiced stops /b/, /d/, /g/ to ‘get’ voiced and voiceless stops. 3. Target affricates to ‘get’ affricates and fricatives. 4. Target clusters to ‘get’ clusters and singletons. ‘get’ means to ‘facilitate generalisation to…’ Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

We can’t look at a consonant and “figure out” whether it is marked. We

We can’t look at a consonant and “figure out” whether it is marked. We simply need to “know” the information. Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP) Can you say these? [����� ] [� � ��� ]

Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP) Can you say these? [����� ] [� � ��� ] Can you say these? [����� ] [�� � �� ] Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

sonority theory � Sonority is the amount of stricture or ‘sound’ in a consonant

sonority theory � Sonority is the amount of stricture or ‘sound’ in a consonant or vowel. � Steriade (1990) proposed a numerical hierarchy. Most sonorant vowels = 0 glides = 1 liquids = 2 nasals = 3 voiced fricatives = 4 voiceless fricatives = 5 voiced stops = 6 voiceless stops = 7 Least sonorant Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

sonority theory We “prefer” to articulate words with a rise and fall in sonority;

sonority theory We “prefer” to articulate words with a rise and fall in sonority; p ���� Most ‘sound’ or starts with the least sonorous stricture in the middle. segment, a voiceless stop, /p/ followed by a liquid, /� / with a vowel, /� / or /� / at the peak, to the less sonorous nasal, /� / finally falling to the least sonorous voiceless stop, /� /. Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

This rise - fall tendency is called the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP) [����� ]

This rise - fall tendency is called the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP) [����� ] and [�� � �� ] are more ‘natural’ for us to say than [����� ] and [� � ��� ] Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

All clusters are marked, but are some clusters more marked than others? Copyright ©

All clusters are marked, but are some clusters more marked than others? Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

One approach to classifying two-element clusters in terms of markedness is to rank them

One approach to classifying two-element clusters in terms of markedness is to rank them according to their sonority difference scores. e. g. , /kw/ (7 minus 1) sonority difference score of 6 /fl/ (5 minus 2) sonority difference score of 3 /fl/ 3 is more marked than /kw/ 6 Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

Sonority difference scores 2 - element clusters with SMALL sonority differences of 2, 3

Sonority difference scores 2 - element clusters with SMALL sonority differences of 2, 3 or 4, and 3 -element clusters, may better promote generalised change to singletons and clusters. § Gierut and co-workers provide evidence and target selection guidelines. § If we work on the more complex, more marked clusters the “others” may emerge without being directly targeted. § Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

Sonority difference scores 2 - element clusters with SMALL sonority differences of 2, 3

Sonority difference scores 2 - element clusters with SMALL sonority differences of 2, 3 or 4, and 3 -element And we get singletons without clusters, may better promote generalised them change targeting to singletons anddirectly! clusters. § Gierut and co-workers provide evidence and target selection guidelines. § If we work on the more complex, more marked clusters the “others” may emerge without being directly targeted. § Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

What’s missing? Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

What’s missing? Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen

Adjuncts /st/, /sp/and /sk/ Morrisette, Farris & Gierut postulate that initial /s/+ stop ‘clusters’

Adjuncts /st/, /sp/and /sk/ Morrisette, Farris & Gierut postulate that initial /s/+ stop ‘clusters’ are adjuncts and not ‘true clusters’, and therefore are not subject to the implicational relationships amongst clusters with respect to sonority, and generalisation. § Meaning, if you target adjuncts you will ‘get’ adjuncts only. § Copyright © 2011 Caroline Bowen