Morphology Words and their Parts CS 4705 Julia

  • Slides: 32
Download presentation
Morphology: Words and their Parts CS 4705 Julia Hirschberg CS 4705

Morphology: Words and their Parts CS 4705 Julia Hirschberg CS 4705

Words • In formal languages, words are arbitrary strings • In natural languages, words

Words • In formal languages, words are arbitrary strings • In natural languages, words are made up of meaningful subunits called morphemes – Morphemes are abstract concepts denoting entities or relationships – Morphemes may be • • Stems: the main morpheme of the word Affixes: convey the word’s role, number, gender, etc. cats == cat [stem] + s [suffix] undo == un [prefix] + do [stem]

Why do we need to do Morphological Analysis? • The study of how words

Why do we need to do Morphological Analysis? • The study of how words are composed from smaller, meaning-bearing units (morphemes) • Applications: – Spelling correction: referece – Hyphenation algorithms: refer-ence – Part-of-speech analysis: googler [N], googling [V] – Text-to-speech: grapheme-to-phoneme conversion • hothouse (/T/ or /D/)

– Let’s us guess the meaning of unknown words • ‘Twas brillig and the

– Let’s us guess the meaning of unknown words • ‘Twas brillig and the slithy toves… • Muggles moogled migwiches

Morphotactics • What are the ‘rules’ for constructing a word in a given language?

Morphotactics • What are the ‘rules’ for constructing a word in a given language? – Pseudo-intellectual vs. *intellectual-pseudo – Rational-ize vs *ize-rational – Cretin-ous vs. *cretin-ly vs. *cretin-acious • Possible ‘rules’ – Suffixes are suffixes and prefixes are prefixes – Certain affixes attach to certain types of stems (nouns, verbs, etc. ) – Certain stems can/cannot take certain affixes

 • Semantics: In English, un- cannot attach to adjectives that already have a

• Semantics: In English, un- cannot attach to adjectives that already have a negative connotation: – Unhappy vs. *unsad – Unhealthy vs. *unsick – Unclean vs. *undirty • Phonology: In English, -er cannot attach to words of more than two syllables – great, greater – Happy, happier – Competent, *competenter – Elegant, *eleganter – Unruly, ? unrulier

Regular and Irregular Morphology • Regular – Walk, walks, walking, walked, (had) walked –

Regular and Irregular Morphology • Regular – Walk, walks, walking, walked, (had) walked – Table, tables • Irregular – Eat, eats, eating, ate, (had) eaten – Catch, catches, catching, caught, (had) caught – Cut, cuts, cutting, cut, (had) cut – Goose, geese

Morphological Parsing • Algorithms developed to use regularities -- and known irregularities -- to

Morphological Parsing • Algorithms developed to use regularities -- and known irregularities -- to parse words into their morphemes • Cats cat +N +PL • Cat cat +N +SG • Cities city +N +PL • Merging merge +V +Present-participle • Caught catch +V +past-participle

Morphology and Finite State Automata • We can use the machinery provided by FSAs

Morphology and Finite State Automata • We can use the machinery provided by FSAs to capture facts about morphology • Accept strings that are in the language • Reject strings that are not • Do this in a way that does not require us to list all the words in the language

How do we build a Morphological Analyzer? • Lexicon: list of stems and affixes

How do we build a Morphological Analyzer? • Lexicon: list of stems and affixes (w/ corresponding part of speech (p. o. s. )) • Morphotactics of the language: model of how and which morphemes can be affixed to a stem • Orthographic rules: spelling modifications that may occur when affixation occurs – in il in context of l (in- + legal) • Most morphological phenomena can be described with regular expressions – so finite state techniques often used to represent morphological processes

Some Simple Rules • Regular singular nouns stay as is • Regular plural nouns

Some Simple Rules • Regular singular nouns stay as is • Regular plural nouns have an -s on the end • Irregulars stay as is

Simple English NP FSA

Simple English NP FSA

Expand the Arcs with Stems and Affixes dog cat geese child

Expand the Arcs with Stems and Affixes dog cat geese child

Parsing/Generation vs. Recognition • We can now run strings through these machines to recognize

Parsing/Generation vs. Recognition • We can now run strings through these machines to recognize strings in the language • Accept words that are ok • Reject words that are not • But is this enough? • We often want to know the structure of a word (understanding/parsing) • Or we may have a stem and want to produce a surface form (production/generation) • Example • From “cats” to “cat +N +PL” • From “cat + N + PL” to “cats”

Finite State Transducers (FSTs) • Turning an FSA into an FST • Add another

Finite State Transducers (FSTs) • Turning an FSA into an FST • Add another tape • Add extra symbols to the transitions • On one tape we read “cats” -- on the other we write “cat +N +PL” • Or vice versa…

Koskenniemi 2 -level Morphology Kimmo Koskenniemi’s two-level morphology Idea: a word is a relationship

Koskenniemi 2 -level Morphology Kimmo Koskenniemi’s two-level morphology Idea: a word is a relationship between lexical level (its morphemes) and surface level (its orthography)

Transitions c: c a: a t: t +N: ε +PL: s • c: c

Transitions c: c a: a t: t +N: ε +PL: s • c: c means read a c on one tape and write a c on the other • +N: ε means read a +N symbol on one tape and write nothing on the other • +PL: s means read +PL and write an s

Not So Simple • Of course, its not all as easy as • “cat

Not So Simple • Of course, its not all as easy as • “cat +N +PL” <-> “cats” • What do we do about geese, mice, oxen? • Many spelling/pronunciation changes go along with inflectional changes, e. g. • Fox and Foxes

Multi-Tape Machines • Solution for complex changes: – Add more tapes – Use output

Multi-Tape Machines • Solution for complex changes: – Add more tapes – Use output of one tape machine as input to the next • To handle irregular spelling changes, add intermediate tapes with intermediate symbols

Example of a Multi-Tape Machine • We use one machine to transduce between the

Example of a Multi-Tape Machine • We use one machine to transduce between the lexical and the intermediate level, and another to transduce between the intermediate and the surface tapes

FST Fragment: Lexical to Intermediate • ^ is morpheme boundary; # is word boundary

FST Fragment: Lexical to Intermediate • ^ is morpheme boundary; # is word boundary

FST Fragment: Intermediate to Surface • Rule: insert an e after a morpheme-final x,

FST Fragment: Intermediate to Surface • Rule: insert an e after a morpheme-final x, s or z and before morpheme s, eg. fox^s# foxes

Putting Them Together

Putting Them Together

Practical Uses • This kind of parsing is normally called morphological analysis • Can

Practical Uses • This kind of parsing is normally called morphological analysis • Can be • An important stand-alone component of an application (spelling correction, information retrieval, part-of-speech tagging, …) • Or simply a link in a chain of processing (machine translation, parsing, …)

Porter Stemmer (1980) • Standard, very popular and usable stemmer (IR, IE) – identify

Porter Stemmer (1980) • Standard, very popular and usable stemmer (IR, IE) – identify a word’s stem • Sequence of cascaded rewrite rules, e. g. – IZE ε (e. g. unionize union) – CY T (e. g. frequency frequent) – ING ε , if stem contains vowel (motoring motor) • Can be implemented as a lexicon-free FST (many implementations available on the web)

Important Note: Morphology Differs by Language • Languages differ in how they encode morphological

Important Note: Morphology Differs by Language • Languages differ in how they encode morphological information – Isolating languages (e. g. Cantonese) have no affixes: each word usually has 1 morpheme – Agglutinative languages (e. g. Finnish, Turkish) are composed of prefixes and suffixes added to a stem (like beads on a string) – each feature realized by a single affix, e. g. Finnish epäjärjestelmällistyttämättömyydellänsäkäänköhän ‘Wonder if he can also. . . with his capability of not causing things to be unsystematic’

– Polysynthetic languages (e. g. Inuit languages) express much of their syntax in their

– Polysynthetic languages (e. g. Inuit languages) express much of their syntax in their morphology, incorporating a verb’s arguments into the verb, e. g. Western Greenlandic Aliikusersuillammassuaanerartassagaluarpaalli. aliiku-sersu-i-llammas-sua-a-nerar-ta-ssa-galuar-paal-li entertainment-provide-SEMITRANS-one. good. at-COPsay. that-REP-FUT-sure. but-3. PL. SUBJ/3 SG. OBJ-but 'However, they will say that he is a great entertainer, but. . . ' – So…. different languages may require very different morphological analyzers

Concatenative vs. Non-concatenative Morphology • Semitic root-and-pattern morphology – Root (2 -4 consonants) conveys

Concatenative vs. Non-concatenative Morphology • Semitic root-and-pattern morphology – Root (2 -4 consonants) conveys basic semantics (e. g. Arabic /ktb/) – Vowel pattern conveys voice and aspect – Derivational template (binyan) identifies word class

Template CVCVC CVCCVC CVVCVC t. VCVVCVC n. CVVCVC Ct. VCVC st. VCCVC Vowel Pattern

Template CVCVC CVCCVC CVVCVC t. VCVVCVC n. CVVCVC Ct. VCVC st. VCCVC Vowel Pattern active passive katab kutib write kattab kuttib cause to write ka: tab ku: tib correspond taka: tab tuku: tib write each other nka: tab nku: tib subscribe ktatab ktutib write staktab stuktib dictate

Morphological Representations: Evidence from Human Performance • Hypotheses: – Full listing hypothesis: words listed

Morphological Representations: Evidence from Human Performance • Hypotheses: – Full listing hypothesis: words listed – Minimum redundancy hypothesis: morphemes listed • Experimental evidence: – Priming experiments (Does seeing/hearing one word facilitate recognition of another? ) suggest something in between • Regularly inflected forms (e. g. cars) prime stem (car) but not derived forms (e. g. management, manage)

 • But spoken derived words can prime stems if they are semantically close

• But spoken derived words can prime stems if they are semantically close (e. g. government/govern but not department/depart) • Speech errors suggest affixes must be represented separately in the mental lexicon – ‘easy enoughly’ for ‘easily enough’ • Importance of morphological family size – Larger families faster recognition

Summing Up • Regular expressions and FSAs can represent subsets of natural language as

Summing Up • Regular expressions and FSAs can represent subsets of natural language as well as regular languages – Both representations may be difficult for humans to understand for any real subset of a language – Can be hard to scale up: e. g. , when many choices at any point (e. g. surnames) – But quick, powerful and easy to use for small problems – AT&T Finite State Toolkit does scale • Next class: – Read Ch 4 on Ngrams – HW 1 will be due at midnight on Oct 1