Lilienfeld S O 2007 Psychological treatments that cause

  • Slides: 31
Download presentation

Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2,

Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 53 -70.

Westen, D. , Blagov, P. S. , Harenski, K. , Kilts, C. , &

Westen, D. , Blagov, P. S. , Harenski, K. , Kilts, C. , & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural bases of motivated reasoning: An f. MRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U. S. presidential election. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1947 -1958.

Norton, M. I. , Mochon, D. , & Ariely, D. (2012). The IKEA effect:

Norton, M. I. , Mochon, D. , & Ariely, D. (2012). The IKEA effect: When labor leads to love. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 453 -460.

Huxham, M. & Land, R. (2000). Assigning students in group work projects: Can we

Huxham, M. & Land, R. (2000). Assigning students in group work projects: Can we do better than random? Innovations in Education and Training International, 37(1), 17 -22. Michaelsen, L. , Knight, A. , and Fink, L. (eds) (2004). Team-based learning: A transformative use of small groups in college teaching, Sterling, VA: Stylus Publishing

Stasser, G. , & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision

Stasser, G. , & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision making: Biased information sampling during discussion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 81 -93

Bowers, C. , Pharmer, J. , and Salas, E. (2000). When member homogeneity is

Bowers, C. , Pharmer, J. , and Salas, E. (2000). When member homogeneity is needed in work teams: A meta-analysis. Small Group Research, 31(3), 305 -327. Hassanien, A. (2006). Student experience of group work and group assessment in higher education. Journal of Teaching in Travel and Tourism, 6(1), 17 -39. Lehman, M. (2007). Influence of learning style heterogeneity on cooperative learning. NACTA Journal, 51(4), 17 -22.

 • Problem solving is often easier when framed by supportive, concrete context (Nisbett

• Problem solving is often easier when framed by supportive, concrete context (Nisbett & Ross, 1980; Wason & Shapiro, 1977) • Authentic, immersive scenarios can be highly motivating and less intimidating (Edelson & Reiser, 2006, Grigorenko, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2002), and more meaningful (Barab & Roth, 2006; Ladson-Billings, 1995) • Contexts bring in useful constraints for improving performance (Mc. Neil Uttal, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2009; Nunes, Schliemann, & Carraher, 1993; Verschaffel et al. , 2000) • Concrete cases are often more memorable (Paivio, 1990) • Visual processes used for concrete objects can be co-opted for abstract reasoning (Glenberg et al. , 2004; Goldstone & Barsalou, 1998) • Concrete details are not always superficial, but are cues as to deep principles (Bassok, 1996)

 • Knowledge is less tied to a specific domain (De. Loache, 1995; 2000)

• Knowledge is less tied to a specific domain (De. Loache, 1995; 2000) • Critical essence of a phenomenon is highlighted as distracting details are eliminated (Goldstone & Sakamoto, 2003; Kaminski, Sloutsky, & Heckler, 2008) • Hard to interpret something as both an object and a symbol (Uttal, Scudder, & De. Loache, 1997) • Extraneous cognitive load of details (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003)

The world of lamp design and retail is competitive and cut-throat. Imagine that you

The world of lamp design and retail is competitive and cut-throat. Imagine that you are invited to the 50 th annual meeting of the “Lamp Designers Guild” that happens to be held in Bloomington, Indiana. You are invited to this convention as an expert statistician to help settle a fight that has broken out among the attendees. . . Often times, it is useful to have a summary characterization for a set of data. Imagine, for example, trying to come up with an apt summary for the number of quality assessments passed by a set of lamps. Imagine that you are invited into a classroom as an expert statistician to help settle a fight that has broken out in the classroom. The classroom is Mrs. Perkins’ sixth grade pre-algebra class in Bloomington, Indiana. The fight has to do what score best represents the class’ overall performance on a mathematics exam. Your task is to address the entire class, and give it advice on which of three possibilities is best. Often times, it is useful to have a summary characterization for a set of data. Imagine, for example, trying to come up with an apt summary for the number of correct answers to a mathematics exam in a small classroom.

Improvement (Posttest - Pretest) 0. 5 Rich Idealized 0. 4 0. 3 0. 2

Improvement (Posttest - Pretest) 0. 5 Rich Idealized 0. 4 0. 3 0. 2 0. 1 0 Intra-Domain Extra-Domain

Motz & Goldstone (in prep. )

Motz & Goldstone (in prep. )

Dickson, K. L. , Miller, M. D. , & Devoley, M. S. (2005). Effect

Dickson, K. L. , Miller, M. D. , & Devoley, M. S. (2005). Effect of textbook study guides on student performance in introductory psychology. Teaching of Psychology, 32, 34 -39