Development in Childrens Written Grammar from Key Stages

  • Slides: 23
Download presentation
Development in Children’s Written Grammar from Key Stages 1 -4 Phil Durrant

Development in Children’s Written Grammar from Key Stages 1 -4 Phil Durrant

Thanks to… • Mark Brenchley (Cambridge Assessment) • Rebecca Clarkson (University of Exeter)

Thanks to… • Mark Brenchley (Cambridge Assessment) • Rebecca Clarkson (University of Exeter)

The Growth in Grammar Project • Aimed to understand written language development through the

The Growth in Grammar Project • Aimed to understand written language development through the course of statutory education in England. • Established a corpus of approximately 3, 000 texts, written by 1, 000 children in Key Stages 1 -4 at 24 schools across the country.

Today’s focus • How do children use adverbial clauses? • How does this use

Today’s focus • How do children use adverbial clauses? • How does this use vary as they progress through school?

Sampled corpus Literary Non-literary Texts Words/text Year 2 30 56 30 84 Year 6

Sampled corpus Literary Non-literary Texts Words/text Year 2 30 56 30 84 Year 6 30 214 30 316 Year 9 30 280 30 350 Year 11 30 328 30 343

Coding Mean inter-coder agreement: status =. 96; pos =. 94; dep =. 89; dep_on

Coding Mean inter-coder agreement: status =. 96; pos =. 94; dep =. 89; dep_on =. 92

Prominence of subordinate clause types Mean frequency/100 words Adverbial Object Relative Noun complement Adjective

Prominence of subordinate clause types Mean frequency/100 words Adverbial Object Relative Noun complement Adjective complement Comparative Subject 0 0, 5 1 Finite 1, 5 Non-finite 2 2, 5 3

Previous findings on adverbials All Adverbial Clauses Source Ages Genre Measure Finding Harpin (1976)

Previous findings on adverbials All Adverbial Clauses Source Ages Genre Measure Finding Harpin (1976) Year 3 vs 6 creative & factive Adv/subordinate clause increase (descriptive) Noyce & Christie Grade 3 vs 5 (1985) mixed Adv/t-unit increase (descriptive) Nippold et al (2005) Grades 5/6 vs 11/12 vs adult persuasive Adv/utterance no significant differences Sampson (2003) 9 -12 years vs adult mixed % adv tagmas decrease (significant)

Previous findings on adverbials Finite Adverbial Clauses Source Ages Genres Measure Finding O’Donnell et

Previous findings on adverbials Finite Adverbial Clauses Source Ages Genres Measure Finding O’Donnell et al (1967) Grade 3 vs 5 vs 7 narrative F. Adv/100 t-units 3<5=7 (significant) Thompson et al (1967) Grade 3 vs 6 narrative F. Adv/100 t-units increase (descriptive) Blount et al (1969) Grade 8 vs 12 mixed F. Adv/1000 word sample decrease (descriptive) Hunt (1965) Grade 4 vs 8 vs 12 mixed F. Adv/1000 word sample no significant differences Golub & Frederick (1970) Grade 4 vs 6 mixed A. Adv/text no significant differences

Previous findings on adverbials Adverbials clauses and genre Source Ages Genres Measure Finding Berninger

Previous findings on adverbials Adverbials clauses and genre Source Ages Genres Measure Finding Berninger et al (2011) Grades 2 -7 narrative & nonnarrative # adv clauses narrative > nonnarrative (significant)

Length: words per adverbial clause All adverbial clauses Finite adverbial clauses MEM: Random intercept

Length: words per adverbial clause All adverbial clauses Finite adverbial clauses MEM: Random intercept for writer Year group: t(190)=6. 1, p<. 0001; Genre: t(190)=4. 1, p<. 0001 R 2 marginal =. 23; R 2 conditional =. 23 Non-Finite adverbial clauses MEM: Random intercept for writer & discipline Year group: t(13)9=4. 2, p<. . 0001; R 2 marginal =. 08; R 2 conditional =. 74

Frequency: adverbials/100 words All adverbial clauses Finite adverbial clauses MEM: Random intercept for title

Frequency: adverbials/100 words All adverbial clauses Finite adverbial clauses MEM: Random intercept for title Year group: t(73)=4. 0, p<. 0005 R 2 marginal =. 11; R 2 conditional =. 37 Non-Finite adverbial clauses MEM: Random intercept for title Year group: t(45)=6. 6, p<. 0001; Genre: t(46)=3. 1, p<. 005 R 2 marginal =. 25; R 2 conditional =. 37

Interim conclusions Age effects • Finite and non-finite clauses are more frequent and longer

Interim conclusions Age effects • Finite and non-finite clauses are more frequent and longer with age • Finite frequency: jumps from Year 2 to Year 6, then levels off • Non-finite frequency: linear increase Genre effects • Non-finite clauses are more frequent in literary than non-literary texts • Finite clauses are longer in non-literary than literary texts

Uses of adverbial clauses in children’s writing Scope-defining Explanatory/evidential Comparison condition conclusion contrast co-occurrence

Uses of adverbial clauses in children’s writing Scope-defining Explanatory/evidential Comparison condition conclusion contrast co-occurrence location text deixis expansion interpretation method reason Interpersonal hedge likeness equivalence correlation interaction

Frequencies per 100 words 1, 8 1, 6 1, 4 1, 2 1 0,

Frequencies per 100 words 1, 8 1, 6 1, 4 1, 2 1 0, 8 0, 6 0, 4 0, 2 0 co-occurrence reason conclusion expansion likeness condition contrast literary text interpretation non-literary method interaction equivalence correlation hedge location

Literary Frequencies per 100 words 2, 5 2 1, 5 1 0, 5 0

Literary Frequencies per 100 words 2, 5 2 1, 5 1 0, 5 0 co-occurrence reason conclusion expansion likeness condition contrast year 2 equivalence year 6 year 9 interaction year 11 hedge interpretation location method correlation text

Non-Literary Frequencies per 100 words 1, 6 1, 4 1, 2 1 0, 8

Non-Literary Frequencies per 100 words 1, 6 1, 4 1, 2 1 0, 8 0, 6 0, 4 0, 2 0 reason co-occurrence conclusion condition contrast text expansion interpretation method year 2 year 11 year 6 year 9 correlation likeness interaction equivalence hedge location

Reasons in non-literary writing (Year 11) • In conclusion, Priestley describes the upper class

Reasons in non-literary writing (Year 11) • In conclusion, Priestley describes the upper class capitalists as arrogant people unwilling to change or help others to try and persuade the audience that capitalism is bad. • This doesn’t support the hypothesis because we need to find out how long it takes hot water to cool. • The “rings of grain” brings a hint of the idea of death as the rings can only be seen when the tree is cut down.

Conclusions: Overall adverbial clause • Adverbials are the most frequent type of subordinate clause

Conclusions: Overall adverbial clause • Adverbials are the most frequent type of subordinate clause in school writing. • Their use can be summarized by a 15 -category taxonomy, under the broad headings of scope defining, explanatory/evidential, comparison, and interpersonal.

Conclusions: Genre-distinctions • In literary texts: • non-finite clauses are more frequent than in

Conclusions: Genre-distinctions • In literary texts: • non-finite clauses are more frequent than in non-literary • the most frequent use of adverbials is to show co-occurrence. • In non-literary texts: • In non-literary texts, finite clauses are longer than in literary texts • the most frequent use of adverbials is to give reasons.

Conclusions: Development • As children progress through school: • they use longer adverbials •

Conclusions: Development • As children progress through school: • they use longer adverbials • they use more adverbials (quickly levelling-off for finite clauses) • Year 2 children use finite adverbials for a narrow range of highfrequency functions and use is less ‘genre-appropriate’. • Genre-specific balance of functions shifts throughout school years, in a non-linear way. • Year 11 writing is particularly strongly marked by an increase in reason clauses in non-literary writing.

References • Berninger, V. W. , Nagy, W. E. , & Beers, S. (2011).

References • Berninger, V. W. , Nagy, W. E. , & Beers, S. (2011). Child writers' construction and reconstruction of single sentences and construction of multi-sentence texts: contributions of syntax and transcription to translation. Reading and Writing, 24, 151 -182. • Golub, L. S. , & Frederick, W. , C. (1979). Linguistic Structures in the discourse of fourth and sixth graders. Madison, Wisconsin: Center for Cognitive Learning, The University of Wisconsin. • Harpin, W. S. (1976). The Second 'R': Writing development in the junior school. London: Taylor and Francis. • Nippold, M. A. , Ward-Lonergan, J. M. , & Fanning, J. L. (2005). Persuasive writing in children, adolescents, and adults: A study of syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic development. Language, speech and hearing services in schools, 36(2), 125138. • Noyce, R. , & Christie, J. (1985). Characteristics of subordinate clauses in children's free writing. Journal of research and development in education, 18(68 -71). • O'Donnell, R. C. , Griffin, W. J. , & Norris, R. C. (1967). A transformational analysis of oral and written grammatical structures in the language of children in grades three, five, and seven. The Journal of Educational Research, 61(1), 35 -39. • Sampson, G. (2003). The structure of children's writing: moving from spoken to adult written norms. Language and computers, 48(1), 177 -193. • Thompson, N. C. , Nemanich, D. D. , & Bala, A. S. (1967). The Nebraska study of the syntax of children's writing, 1966 -67. Volume III. Nebraska: The University of Nebraska.

Further information and access to corpus Please contact: p. l. durrant@exeter. ac. uk

Further information and access to corpus Please contact: p. l. durrant@exeter. ac. uk