Confounding in epidemiology Maura Pugliatti MD Ph D

  • Slides: 58
Download presentation
Confounding in epidemiology Maura Pugliatti, MD, Ph. D Associate Professor of Neurology Dept. of

Confounding in epidemiology Maura Pugliatti, MD, Ph. D Associate Professor of Neurology Dept. of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Unit of Clinical Neurology University of Sassari, Italy 1 st International Course of Neuroepidemiology Chisinau, Moldova, 24 -28 Sept. 2012

Definitions “Confounding, the situation in which an apparent effect of an exposure on risk

Definitions “Confounding, the situation in which an apparent effect of an exposure on risk is explained by its association with other factors, is probably the most important cause of spurious associations in observational epidemiology” BMJ Editorial: “The scandal of poor epidemiological research” BMJ 2004; 329: 868 -869 “Bias of the estimated effect of an exposure on an outcome, due to the presence of a common cause of the exposure and the outcome” Porta, 2008

Overview Causality: central concern of epidemiology Confounding: central concern when establishing causality Four approaches

Overview Causality: central concern of epidemiology Confounding: central concern when establishing causality Four approaches to understand confounding Avoiding and controlling for confounding is essential in health research

Causality Main application of epidemiology: to identify etiologic (causal) associations between exposure(s) and outcome(s)

Causality Main application of epidemiology: to identify etiologic (causal) associations between exposure(s) and outcome(s) ? Exposure Outcome

Key biases in identifying causal effects: Causal Effect Random Error Confounding Information bias (misclassification)

Key biases in identifying causal effects: Causal Effect Random Error Confounding Information bias (misclassification) Selection bias Bias in inference Reporting & publication bias Bias in knowledge use RRcausal “truth” RRassociation Adapted from: Maclure, M, Schneeweis S. Epidemiology 2001; 12: 114 -122.

Confounding: four approaches 1. “Mixing of effects” 2. Based on a priori criteria (classical

Confounding: four approaches 1. “Mixing of effects” 2. Based on a priori criteria (classical approach) 3. Data-based criteria 4. “Counterfactual” and non-comparability approaches Overlapping

“Confounding is confusion, or mixing, of effects; the effect of the exposure is mixed

“Confounding is confusion, or mixing, of effects; the effect of the exposure is mixed together with the effect of another variable, leading to bias” Latin: “confundere” = “to mix together” Rothman KJ. Epidemiology. An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002

Association between birth order and Down Syndrome Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

Association between birth order and Down Syndrome Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

Association between maternal age and Down Syndrome Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

Association between maternal age and Down Syndrome Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

Association between maternal age and Down Syndrome, stratified by birth order Data from Stark

Association between maternal age and Down Syndrome, stratified by birth order Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

A factor is a confounder if 3 criteria are met: 1. A confounder must

A factor is a confounder if 3 criteria are met: 1. A confounder must be causally or non-causally associated with the exposure in the source population (study base) being studied; C E 2. A confounder must be a causal risk factor (or a C surrogate measure of a cause) for the disease in the unexposed cohort; and 3. A confounder must not be an intermediate cause (not an intermediate step in the causal pathway between the exposure and the disease) E C X D D

Confounder C Exposure Disease (outcome) D E Exposure E Intermediate cause C Disease D

Confounder C Exposure Disease (outcome) D E Exposure E Intermediate cause C Disease D Szklo M, Nieto JF. Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. Aspen Publishers, Inc. , 2000. Gordis L. Epidemiology. Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 4 th Edition.

Confounder: ‘parent’ of the exposure not ‘daughter’ of the exposure!!! Exposure Disease E D

Confounder: ‘parent’ of the exposure not ‘daughter’ of the exposure!!! Exposure Disease E D Confounder C

Confounding factor: Maternal Age C Birth Order E Down Syndrome D

Confounding factor: Maternal Age C Birth Order E Down Syndrome D

Simple causal graphs C E D Maternal age (C) can confound the association between

Simple causal graphs C E D Maternal age (C) can confound the association between multivitamin use (E) and the risk of certain birth defects (D) Hernan MA, et al. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol 2002; 155: 176 -84.

Complex causal graphs U C E D History of birth defects (C) may increase

Complex causal graphs U C E D History of birth defects (C) may increase the chance of periconceptional vitamin intake (E). A genetic factor (U) could have been the cause of previous birth defects in the family, and could again cause birth defects in the current pregnancy (D) Hernan MA, et al. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol 2002; 155: 176 -84.

More complicated causal graphs Physical Activity Smoking A B BMI C U E D

More complicated causal graphs Physical Activity Smoking A B BMI C U E D Calcium supplementation Bone fractures Source: Hertz-Picciotto

A factor is a confounder if: a) the effect measure is homogeneous across the

A factor is a confounder if: a) the effect measure is homogeneous across the strata defined by the confounder and b) the crude and common stratum-specific (adjusted) effect measures are unequal (“lack of collapsibility”) Usually evaluated using 2 x 2 tables, and simple stratified analyses to compare crude effects with adjusted effects “Collapsibility is equality of stratum-specific measures of effect with the crude (collapsed), unstratified measure” Porta, 2008, Dictionary

Crude vs. Adjusted Effects Crude: does not take into account the effect of the

Crude vs. Adjusted Effects Crude: does not take into account the effect of the confounder Adjusted: accounts for the confounder Mantel-Haenszel method estimator Multivariate analyses (e. g. logistic regression) Confounding is likely when: RRcrude =/= RRadjusted ORcrude =/= ORadjusted

Stratified Analysis Crude ORCrude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify

Stratified Analysis Crude ORCrude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify by Confounder Stratum 1 Stratum 2 OR 1 OR 2 Calculate OR’s for each stratum If stratum-specific OR’s are similar, calculate adjusted OR (e. g. MH) If Crude OR =/= Adjusted OR, confounding is likely If Crude OR = Adjusted OR, confounding is unlikely

Ideal “causal contrast” between exposed and unexposed groups: “A causal contrast compares disease frequency

Ideal “causal contrast” between exposed and unexposed groups: “A causal contrast compares disease frequency under two exposure distributions, but in one target population during one etiologic time period” If the ideal causal contrast is met, the observed effect is the “causal effect” Maldonado & Greenland, Int J Epi 2002; 31: 422 -29

Ideal counterfactual comparison to determine causal effects: Exposed cohort Iexp Initial conditions are identical

Ideal counterfactual comparison to determine causal effects: Exposed cohort Iexp Initial conditions are identical in the exposed and unexposed groups, except for presence of exposure (=cause) Unexposed cohort Iunexp RRcausal = Iexp / Iunexp Maldonado & Greenland, Int J Epi 2002; 31: 422 -29

What happens in reality? Exposed cohort Unexposed cohort Substitute, unexposed cohort RRassoc = Iexp

What happens in reality? Exposed cohort Unexposed cohort Substitute, unexposed cohort RRassoc = Iexp / Isubstitute Iexp Iunexp Isubstitute

In this case: RRcausal = Iexp / Iunexp IDEAL RRassoc = Iexp / Isubstitute

In this case: RRcausal = Iexp / Iunexp IDEAL RRassoc = Iexp / Isubstitute ACTUAL “Confounding is present if the substitute population represents imperfectly what the target would have been like under the counterfactual condition”

Simulating the counter-factual comparison: Experimental Studies: Randomized Clinical Trials Eligible population Disease + Treated

Simulating the counter-factual comparison: Experimental Studies: Randomized Clinical Trials Eligible population Disease + Treated individuals Disease - compare rates Randomization Disease + Untreated individuals Disease - Randomization helps to make the groups “comparable” (i. e. similar initial conditions) with respect to known and unknown confounders Confounding is unlikely at randomization - time t 0

Simulating the counter-factual comparison: Observational Studies: Cohort studies, case-control studies Disease + Exposed cohort

Simulating the counter-factual comparison: Observational Studies: Cohort studies, case-control studies Disease + Exposed cohort Disease - compare rates Disease + Unexposed cohort Disease - PRESENT FUTURE In observational studies, because exposures are not assigned randomly, attainment of exchangeability is impossible – “initial conditions” are likely to be different and the groups may not be comparable

Confounding: Observational studies vs randomized trials Example: Aspirin to reduce cardiovascular mortality

Confounding: Observational studies vs randomized trials Example: Aspirin to reduce cardiovascular mortality

Confounding: adjustment and controls • Control at the design stage – Randomization – Restriction

Confounding: adjustment and controls • Control at the design stage – Randomization – Restriction – Matching • Control at the analysis stage – Conventional approaches • Stratified analyses • Multivariate analyses – Newer approaches • • Graphical approaches using DAGs Propensity scores Instrumental variables Marginal structural models

 • Options at the design stage: – Randomization • Reduces potential for confounding

• Options at the design stage: – Randomization • Reduces potential for confounding by generating groups that are fairly comparable with respect to known and unknown confounding variables – Restriction • Eliminates variation in the confounder (e. g. only recruiting one gender) – Matching • Involves selection of a comparison group that is forced to resemble the index group with respect to the distribution of one or more potential confounders

Randomization • Randomization – Only for intervention studies – Definition: random assignment of study

Randomization • Randomization – Only for intervention studies – Definition: random assignment of study subjects to exposure categories – To control/reduce the effect of confounding variables about which the investigator is unaware (i. e. both known and unknown confounders get distributed evenly because of randomization) – Randomization does not always eliminate confounding • Covariate imbalance in small trials • “Maldistribution” of potentially confounding variables after randomization (“Table I: Baseline characteristics” in the randomized trial)

Randomization breaks any links between treatment and prognostic factors Confounder C Randomization X Exposure

Randomization breaks any links between treatment and prognostic factors Confounder C Randomization X Exposure E Disease (outcome) D

Restriction • The distribution of the potential confounding factors does not vary across exposure

Restriction • The distribution of the potential confounding factors does not vary across exposure or disease categories – An investigator may restrict study subjects to only those falling with specific level(s) of a confounding variable • Advantages of restriction – straightforward, convenient, inexpensive (but, reduces recruitment!) • Disadvantages of restriction – – Limits number of eligible subjects Limits ability to generalize the study findings Residual confounding Impossible to evaluate the relationship of interest at different levels of the confounder

Matching • Matching is commonly used in case-control studies • Match on strong confounder

Matching • Matching is commonly used in case-control studies • Match on strong confounder • Types: – Pair (individual) matching – Frequency matching • The use of matching usually requires special analysis techniques (e. g. matched pair analyses and conditional logistic regression)

Matching • Disadvantages of matching – Finding appropriate control subjects: difficult and expensive and

Matching • Disadvantages of matching – Finding appropriate control subjects: difficult and expensive and limit sample size – Confounder used to match subjects cannot be evaluated with respect to the outcome/disease – Matching does not control for confounders other than those used to match – The use of matching makes the use of stratified analysis very difficult – Matching is most often used in case-control studies (prohibitive in a large cohort study) – In a case-control study, matching may even introduce confounding

Controlling Confounding: At the analysis stage Conventional approaches

Controlling Confounding: At the analysis stage Conventional approaches

Confounding: control at the analysis stage • Confounding is one type of bias that

Confounding: control at the analysis stage • Confounding is one type of bias that can be adjusted in the analysis (unlike selection and information bias) • Options at the analysis stage: – Stratification – Multivariate methods • To control for confounding in the analyses, confounders must be measured in the study

Stratification • Produce groups within which the confounder does not vary • Evaluate the

Stratification • Produce groups within which the confounder does not vary • Evaluate the exposure-disease association within each stratum of the confounder

Source: www. epiet. org

Source: www. epiet. org

Stratified Analysis Crude ORCrude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify

Stratified Analysis Crude ORCrude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify by Confounder Stratum 1 Stratum 2 OR 1 OR 2 Calculate OR’s for each stratum If stratum-specific OR’s are similar, calculate adjusted OR (e. g. MH) If Crude OR =/= Adjusted OR, confounding is likely If Crude OR = Adjusted OR, confounding is unlikely

Direction of Confounding • Confounding “pulls” the observed association away from the true association

Direction of Confounding • Confounding “pulls” the observed association away from the true association – It can either exaggerate/over-estimate the true association (positive confounding) • Example – ORcausal = 1. 0 – ORobserved = 3. 0 or – It can hide/under-estimate the true association (negative confounding) • Example – ORcausal = 3. 0 – ORobserved = 1. 0

Multivariate Analysis • Stratified analysis works best only in the presence of 1 or

Multivariate Analysis • Stratified analysis works best only in the presence of 1 or 2 confounders • If the number of potential confounders is large, multivariate analyses offer the only real solution – Can handle large numbers of confounders (covariates) simultaneously – Based on statistical regression “models” • E. g. logistic regression, multiple linear regression – Always done with statistical software packages

Residual confounding • Confounding that can persist, even after adjustment – Unmeasured confounding –

Residual confounding • Confounding that can persist, even after adjustment – Unmeasured confounding – Some variables were actually not confounders – Confounders were measured with error (eg. , misclassification) – Categories of the confounder improperly defined

44

44

Effect modification and interaction Maura Pugliatti, MD, Ph. D Associate Professor of Neurology Dept.

Effect modification and interaction Maura Pugliatti, MD, Ph. D Associate Professor of Neurology Dept. of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Unit of Clinical Neurology University of Sassari, Italy 1 st International Course of Neuroepidemiology Chisinau, Moldova, 24 -28 Sept. 2012

Definition ü Biological interaction ü Effect modification (“effect-measure modification”) ü Heterogeneity of effects ü

Definition ü Biological interaction ü Effect modification (“effect-measure modification”) ü Heterogeneity of effects ü Subgroup effects ü Statistical Interaction ü Deviation from a specified model form (additive or multiplicative)

Biological interaction “the interdependent operation of two or more biological causes to produce, prevent

Biological interaction “the interdependent operation of two or more biological causes to produce, prevent or control an effect” [Porta, Dictionary, 2008]

Multicausality and interdependent effects Ø Disease processes tend to be multifactorial: “multicausality” Ø The

Multicausality and interdependent effects Ø Disease processes tend to be multifactorial: “multicausality” Ø The “one-variable-at-a-time” perspective has several limitations Ø Confounding and effect modification: manifestations of multicausality Schoenbach, 2000

Effect modification and statistical interaction ü Two definitions (related): ü Based on homogeneity or

Effect modification and statistical interaction ü Two definitions (related): ü Based on homogeneity or heterogeneity of effects ü Interaction occurs when the effect of a risk factor (X) on an outcome (Y) is not homogeneous in strata formed by a third variable (Z, effect modifier) ü “Differences in the effect measure for one factor at different levels of another factor” [Porta, 2008] ü This is often called “effect modification” ü Based on the comparison between observed and expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable ü Interaction occurs when the observed joint effects of the risk factor (X) and third variable (Z) differs from that expected on the basis of their independent effects ü This is often called “statistical interaction” Szklo & Nieto, Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. 2007

Definition based on homogeneity or heterogeneity of effects Effect of exposure on the disease

Definition based on homogeneity or heterogeneity of effects Effect of exposure on the disease is modified depending on the value of a third variable: the “effect modifier” Effect modifier Exposure Disease

Stratified Analysis Crude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Crude ORCrude

Stratified Analysis Crude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Crude ORCrude Stratify by Confounder Stratum 1 Stratum 2 OR 1 OR 2 Calculate OR’s for each stratum If stratum-specific OR’s are the same or similar, calculate adjusted OR (e. g. MH) If stratum-specific OR’s are not similar, calculate adjusted OR (e. g. MH) Effect modification is present. Report Stratum-specific OR If Crude OR =/= Adjusted OR, confounding is likely. Report Adjusted OR If Crude OR = Adjusted OR, confounding is unlikely. Report Crude OR

Confounding vs. interaction ü Confounding is a problem we want to eliminate (control or

Confounding vs. interaction ü Confounding is a problem we want to eliminate (control or adjust for) in our study ü Comparing crude vs. adjusted effect estimates ü Interaction is a natural occurrence that we want to describe and study further ü Comparing stratum-specific estimates

Heterogeneity of effects ü Can occur at the level of: ü Individual study: within

Heterogeneity of effects ü Can occur at the level of: ü Individual study: within subgroups of a single study or trial ü Seen in subgroup or stratified analyses within a study ü Across studies: if several studies are done on the same topic, the effect measures may vary across studies ü Seen in meta-analyses (across trials)

Definition based on the comparison between observed and expected joint effects of a risk

Definition based on the comparison between observed and expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable Deviation from additive or multiplicative joint effects This is often called “statistical interaction”

Observed vs expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable No

Observed vs expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable No interaction Positive interaction Negative interaction Szklo & Nieto, Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. 2007

Deviation from additive or multiplicative joint effects ü Interaction on an “additive” scale (additive

Deviation from additive or multiplicative joint effects ü Interaction on an “additive” scale (additive interaction) ü Effect measure modification when risk difference is used as measure of effect ü Additive statistical model: ü Linear regression: y = a + b 1 x 1 + b 2 x 2 ü Interaction on a “multiplicative” scale (multiplicative interaction) ü Effect measure modification when risk ratio is used as measure of effect ü Multiplicative statistical model: ü Logistic regression:

Additive or multiplicative model? ü The additive model underpins the methods for assessing biological

Additive or multiplicative model? ü The additive model underpins the methods for assessing biological interaction ü Interaction here is a departure from additivity of disease rates (risk difference is the key measure) ü Risk difference scale is of greatest public health importance (based on attributable risk) ü Many of the models used in epidemiology are inherently multiplicative (e. g. logistic regression) ü Vast majority of epi analyses implicitly use the multiplicative scale (risk ratio is the key measure) ü Because most epi studies report RR and OR estimates and use regression models such as logistic and survival analyses – these models inherently use ratio measures and are therefore multiplicative Ahlbom A et al. Eur J Epi 2005

Why is interaction/effect modification important? ü Better understanding of causation ü Identification of “high-risk”

Why is interaction/effect modification important? ü Better understanding of causation ü Identification of “high-risk” groups ü Target interventions at specific subgroups